Sunday 26 January 2014

human biology - What are the consequences of voluntary total celibacy?

Not entirely on your question but I think sufficiently relevant:



This paper (freely available) identified a non-causal link between ejaculatory frequency and incidence of prostate cancer.




Men were asked their age at first ejaculation, the maximum number of ejaculations ever experienced in 24 h, and to estimate the average number of times that they had ejaculated per week in their most sexually active year in each of three decades of age (i.e. third, fourth and fifth).




Leading to results indicating that:




Greater ejaculatory frequency in the most sexually active year in each of the three decades was associated with a significantly lower risk, with men in the upper quartile of ejaculatory frequency having about two-thirds the risk of those in the lower quartile for the third and fourth decade, and four-fifths the risk for the fifth. Ejaculatory frequencies in each of the decades were correlated with one another (r = 0.5–0.7). A similar finding related to the total number of ejaculations over the three decades. Those who reported an average of four to five or more ejaculations per week had two-thirds the risk compared with those who, on average, ejaculated less than three times per week.





Giles, G.G., Severi, G., English, D.R., McCredie, M.R.E., Borland, R., Boyle, P. and Hopper, J.L. (2003), Sexual factors and prostate cancer. BJU International, 92: 211–216. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-410X.2003.04319.x

Saturday 25 January 2014

evolution - Is there are evolutionary explanation for why humans and primates are ticklish? How might it have evolved?

Tickling probably evolved from a defense mechanism but then gradually changed into a more social action, as explained in Provine, 2005 (PDF):




The neurological mechanism of tickling probably evolved from a reflex defense mechanism that protects our body’s surface from external, moving stimuli, probably predators or parasites. Our response to tickle is more varied and complex than the typical reflex, but it has some stereotypic, reflex-like properties (i.e., we laugh when tickled, struggle to escape the tickler, huddle, fend off the tickling hand). Although you can be tickled to laughter by a machine (Harris, 1999) (PDF), most everyday tickle is yet another social context for laughter and a form of communication.


Wednesday 22 January 2014

human biology - How does a brain distinguish stimuli?

Just adding some metaphors in support to the excellent answer by walkytalky.



Our brain is like a huge processing center that can be seen from techni's prospective as a sort of a data center with parallel processing of hundreds of thousands of inputs with processing cores scattered throughout the complete brain.



Human brain (medulla) is connected with the rest of the body through special types of nerves that convey the information to brain (sensory, centropetal nerves) or deliver information from brain to the target organs (motor or centrofugal nerves) or both (mixed nerves).



I intentionally use the term "medulla", because this word comes from origin where it is applied to somwwhat more than brain: the spinal cord is also "medulla" (medulla spinalis), as well as the part that connects the spinal cord with the brain itself (medula oblongata).



The nerves that come directly from the brain are called cranial nerves. There are only 12 pairs of them in humans, numbered using Roman numbers (from I to XII) and every pair has a specific function: I bringing olfactory information (about smells), II -- the optic one, III -- moving the eyes etc.



The counterparts of these nerves in spinal cord are spinal nerves, which go through the holes between single vertebrae.



Brain and CNS in general can locate the information input and determine the information type judging by the input source using these nerves.



Besides the classical sensory information, like those from eyes coming via optical nerves into CNS, there is also some sensory information that is conveyed through autonomous nerve system, that is less specific and can not be localized as well as in case of sensory information coming via sensory input. Pain, for example, or the feeling of pressure belongs to the type of information coming via vegetative nerves.



The complete brain surface (so-called cortex) is mapped into special zones, called Brodmann zones, depending upon prevailing neuron types and (secondary) role in information processing.

Tuesday 21 January 2014

human biology - Why is there an extended delay before G.M. liver cells are attacked by the immune system?

The delay in immune attack is common for many viral infectious deceases where auto-immune response plays substantial to significant role in pathogenesis of the decease. The classical example here are the viral hepatitis B, C and D where it is auto-immune attack that causes the massive death of liver cells (hepatocytes).



The delay in immune attack is explained by the following mechanisms:



The immune attack is developed against the surface antigen, in case of hepatitis it is the HbS Antigen. "Surface" means that this antigen has to appear on the cell surface in order to elicit immune response: the virus itself as well as the parts of destroyed cells are not immunogenic enough for this.



The second point explaining why the attack takes so long to develop is that it is primarily cell-mediated attack, the humoral factors play the secondary role here.



So, the usual "workflow" in this case is the following:



  1. Contamination with hepatitis virus.

  2. Virus propagation to hepatic cells and their infection.

  3. Processing of the viral DNA (either as DNA or first reversed-transcripted from RNA) so that it is integrated into cell normal processing pathways.

  4. Expression of viral proteins (=antigenes) and their anchoring in the cell membrane.

  5. Surface antigenes are detected by macrophagues, processed and expressed on their membrane for T-helpers
    ... (continuation of the usual cell-mediated immune response via T4 and T8 lymphocytes).

Every stage takes some time, the total duration can sum up to 4 to 12 weeks depending upon the viral activity and the immune system status.

Wednesday 8 January 2014

zoology - Does artificial high intensity light damage permanently dark ecosystems?

I don't know how harmful the exposure to a camera flash could be to organisms that live in complete darkness. However, since the animals that live in this conditions are usually blind, and the time of exposure is quite short, it's reasonable to assume that the damage produced should be minimal.



In the case of caves, permanent artificial illumination is very destructive to the environment, but the effect is due mainly to temperature changes and the introduction of organisms coming from more illuminated parts of the cave (cave organisms are usually slow and react poorly to light, so they are an easy prey if they're under light). In fact, for tourist activity, the use of lanterns or miner helmets is highly recommended.



In the case of abyssal ecosystems, ecological invasions are less likely, since there still exist a vertical barrier based in temperature, oxygen and carbon dioxide concentration and pressure. However, in this ecosystems there exist many animals which are very sensitive to light, due to the prominence of bioluminescence. Some of this animals could be damaged by the kind of light you describe. However, it's very unlikely that the main functioning of this ecosystems could be affected because of its extensive size. Moreover, the low population densities it has implies that even very long submersions would encounter only a few samples (with the exception of volcanic vents and some other geological curiosities which, in fact, are even more isolated from the rest of the biosphere by physical and chemical barriers), so the global effect should be negligible.

Friday 3 January 2014

neuroscience - Are there neurons that can sense light shining in your ears?

There is no known mechanism for light detection through the ears in humans, as far as I know. It is certainly true that the pineal gland is part of the system that regulates the circadian rhythm (briefly, the daily sleep-wake cycle). However, while the pineal gland in birds and other non-mammalian vertebrates is directly sensitive to light, the mammalian pineal gland is not (see, for review, Doyle and Menaker, 2007 and Csernus, 2006).



In all animals, the circadian rhythm is regulated by a photoperiod cue and therefore requires light detection. In mammals, the light sensors are found exclusively in the retina, the sensory portion of the eye. There are two classes of light detecting cells in the retina. First, rod and cone photoreceptors mediate vision in the usual sense of the word. These cells contain proteins called opsins that absorb photons of light and thereby excite the photoreceptors that contain them, informing the brain that light was detected.



A second class of photosenstive cells in the retina are called intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) (see Do and Yau, 2010 for review). These cells mediate "non-image-forming" vision and are an important part of the circadian rhythm pathway. They also contain an opsin called melanopsin which is a photosensitive pigment. This is not to be confused with melatonin, which is the sleep hormone released by the pineal gland. The ipRGCs in the retina send the photoperiod cue to a brain area called the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). The SCN then signals to the pineal gland.



If we are generous and assume that these light-emitting headphones are the result of misunderstandings, we can guess that the confusion arises from (1) the fact that some animals have a directly photosensitive pineal gland, but not mammals and (2) that the pineal gland secretes melatonin but not the photosensitive pigment melanopsin.




Update: From a bit of research, it turns out that the company selling the headphones is not "confused" as I politely offered. I don't think this site is the appropriate forum to refute their research or claims. Suffice to say that the retina is the only part of the human brain shown to be photosensitive.